单选题
Why the Mona Lisa Stands Out
A. Have you ever fallen for a novel and been amazed not to find it on lists of great books? Or walked around a sculpture renowned as a classic, struggling to see what the fuss is about? If so, you"ve probably pondered the question a psychologist, James Cutting, asked himself: How does a work of art come to be considered great?
B. The intuitive answer is that some works of art are just great—of intrinsically superior quality. The paintings that win prime spots in galleries, get taught in classes and reproduced in books are the ones that have proved their artistic value over time. If you can"t see they"re superior, that"s your problem. It"s an intimidatingly neat explanation. But some social scientists have been asking awkward questions of it, raising the possibility that artistic canons (名作目录) are little more than fossilised historical accidents.
C. Cutting, a professor at Cornell University, wondered if a psychological mechanism known as the "mere-exposure effect" played a role in deciding which paintings rise to the top of the cultural league. Cutting designed an experiment to test his hunch (直觉). Over a lecture course he regularly showed undergraduates works of impressionism for two seconds at a time. Some of the paintings were canonical, included in art-history books. Others were lesser known but of comparable quality. These were exposed four times as often. Afterwards, the students preferred them to the canonical works, while a control group of students liked the canonical ones best. Cutting"s students had grown to like those paintings more simply because they had seen them more.
D. Cutting believes his experiment offers a clue as to how canons are formed. He points out that the most reproduced works of impressionism today tend to have been bought by five or six wealthy and influential collectors in the late 19th century. The preferences of these men bestowed (给予) prestige on certain works, which made the works more likely to be hung in galleries and printed in collections. The fame passed down the years, gaining momentum from mere exposure as it did so. The more people were exposed to, the more they liked it, and the more they liked it, the more it appeared in books, on posters and in big exhibitions. Meanwhile, academics and critics created sophisticated justifications for its preeminence (卓越). After all, it"s not just the masses who tend to rate what they see more often more highly. As contemporary artists like Warhol and Damien Hirst have grasped, critics" praise is deeply entwined (交织) with publicity. "Scholars," Cutting argues, "are no different from the public in the effects of mere exposure."
E. The process described by Cutting evokes a principle that the sociologist Duncan Watts calls "cumulative advantage": Once a thing becomes popular, it will tend to become more popular still. A few years ago, Watts, who is employed by Microsoft to study the dynamics of social networks, had a similar experience to Cutting"s in another Paris museum. After queuing to see the Mona Lisa in its climate-controlled bulletproof box at the Louvre, he came away puzzled: Why was it considered so superior to the three other Leonardos in the previous chamber, to which nobody seemed to be paying the slightest attention?
F. When Watts looked into the history of "the greatest painting of all time", he discovered that, for most of its life, the Mona Lisa remained in relative obscurity. In the 1850s, Leonardo da Vinci was considered no match for giants of Renaissance art like Titian and Raphael, whose works were worth almost ten times as much as the Mona Lisa. It was only in the 20th century that Leonardo"s portrait of his patron"s wife rocketed to the number-one spot. What propelled it there wasn"t a scholarly reevaluation, but a theft.
G. In 1911 a maintenance worker at the Louvre walked out of the museum with the Mona Lisa hidden under his smock (工作服). Parisians were shocked at the theft of a painting to which, until then, they had paid little attention. When the museum reopened, people queued to see the gap where the Mona Lisa had once hung in a way they had never done for the painting itself. From then on, the Mona Lisa came to represent Western culture itself.
H. Although many have tried, it does seem improbable that the painting"s unique status can be attributed entirely to the quality of its brushstrokes. It has been said that the subject"s eyes follow the viewer around the room. But as the painting"s biographer, Donald Sassoon, dryly notes, "In reality the effect can be obtained from any portrait." Duncan Watts proposes that the Mona Lisa is merely an extreme example of a general rule. Paintings, poems and pop songs are buoyed (使浮起) or sunk by random events or preferences that turn into waves of influence, passing down the generations.
I. "Saying that cultural objects have value," Brian Eno once wrote, "is like saying that telephones have conversations." Nearly all the cultural objects we consume arrive wrapped in inherited opinion; our preferences are always, to some extent, someone else"s. Visitors to the Mona Lisa know they are about to visit the greatest work of art ever and come away appropriately impressed—or let down. An audience at a performance of Hamlet know it is regarded as a work of genius, so that is what they mostly see. Watts even calls the preeminence of Shakespeare a "historical accident".
J. Although the rigid high-low distinction fell apart in the 1960s, we still use culture as a badge of identity. Today"s fashion for eclecticism (折衷主义)—"I love Bach, Abba and Jay Z"—is, Shamus Khan, a Columbia University psychologist, argues, a new way for the middle class to distinguish themselves from what they perceive to be the narrow tastes of those beneath them in the social hierarchy.
K. The intrinsic quality of a work of art is starting to seem like its least important attribute. But perhaps it"s more significant than our social scientists allow. First of all, a work needs a certain quality to be eligible to be swept to the top of the pile. The Mona Lisa may not be a worthy world champion, but it was in the Louvre in the first place, and not by accident. Secondly, some stuff is simply better than other stuff. Read Hamlet after reading even the greatest of Shakespeare"s contemporaries, and the difference may strike you as unarguable.
L. A study in the British Journal of Aesthetics suggests that the exposure effect doesn"t work the same way on everything, and points to a different conclusion about how canons are formed. The social scientists are right to say that we should be a little sceptical of greatness, and that we should always look in the next room. Great art and mediocrity (平庸) can get confused, even by experts. But that"s why we need to see, and read, as much as we can. The more we"re exposed to the good and the bad, the better we are at telling the difference. The eclecticists have it.
单选题
According to Duncan Watts, the superiority of the Mona Lisa to Leonardo"s other works resulted from the cumulative advantage.
单选题
Some social scientists have raised doubts about the intrinsic value of certain works of art.
【正确答案】
B
【答案解析】B段最后一句提到,有些社会科学家对此提出了发难的质疑,他们提出,艺术作品被收入名作目录可能仅仅是被固化的历史偶然。其中的it即指第1句提到的杰出作品内在品质的卓越性。综合这两句可知,有些社会科学家怀疑杰出作品内在品质的卓越性。题目是对这两句的概括。其中have raised doubts about对应文中的have been asking awkward questions of,value对应quality,certain对应some。
单选题
It is often random events or preferences that determine the fate of a piece of art.
【正确答案】
H
【答案解析】H段最后一句提到,偶然的事件或偏好最终形成舆论影响,使画作、诗歌和流行歌曲命运浮沉,一代代流传下去。由此可见,偶然事件或喜好常常能决定一件艺术作品的命运。题目是对该句的同义转述。其中的a piece of art对应文中的paintings,poems and pop songs。
单选题
In his experiment, Cutting found that his subjects liked lesser known works better than canonical works because of more exposure.
【正确答案】
C
【答案解析】C段第2句提到,卡廷设计了一项试验来检验自己的直觉。接着下文详细介绍了他的实验内容和实验结果,其中第5、6句提到,卡廷在课堂上更多地展示知名度较低的作品,而后,参与试验的学生表示他们更欣赏这些知名度较低的作品。最后一句得出结论:卡廷的学生们之所以会欣赏那些作品仅仅是因为他们看到那些作品的次数更多。综合以上信息可知,卡廷通过试验发现,试验对象更欣赏知名度较低的作品,原因在于它们被曝光的次数更多。题目是对文中多处信息的概括。其中的more exposure对应文中的had seen them more。
单选题
The author thinks the greatness of an artwork still lies in its intrinsic value.
单选题
We need to expose ourselves to more art and literature in order to tell the superior from the inferior.
【正确答案】
B、L
【答案解析】L段倒数第二、三句提到,我们要尽可能地多看、多读,我们见过的好作品和坏作品越多,分辨优劣差异的本领就越强。也就是说,要获得判断优劣的能力,需要多接触艺术和文学作品。题目是对这两句的概括。其中tell the superior from the inferior对应文中的telling the difference。
单选题
A study of the history of the greatest paintings suggests even a great work of art could experience years of neglect.
【正确答案】
F
【答案解析】F段第1句提到,在调查“各个时期最杰出画作”的历史时,瓦特发现在《蒙娜丽莎》存世的大部分时间里,它还相对默默无闻。由此可以推断,即便是伟大的艺术作品,也可能被人忽视多年。题目是对该句的合理推断。其中的a study of对应文中的looked into;years of对应most of its life;neglect对应remained in relative obscurity。
单选题
Culture is still used as a mark to distinguish one social class from another.