单选题
New science reveals how your brain is hard-wired when it comes to spending—and how you can reboot it.
The choice to spend rather than save reflects a very human—and, some would say, American—quirk: a preference for immediate gratification over future gains. In other words, we get far more joy from buying a new pair of shoes today, or a Caribbean vacation, or an iPhone 4S, than from imagining a comfortable life tomorrow. Throw in an instant-access culture—in which we can get answers on the Internet within seconds, have a coffeepot delivered to our door overnight, and watch movies on demand—and we"re not exactly training the next generation to delay gratification. "Pleasure now is worth more to us than pleasure later," says economist William Dickens of Northeastern University. "We much prefer current consumption to future consumption. It may even be wired into us."
As brain scientists plumb the neurology of an afternoon at the mall, they are discovering measurable differences between the brains of people who save and those who spend with abandon, particularly in areas of the brain that predict consequences, process the sense of reward, spur motivation, and control memory. In fact, neuroscientists are mapping the brain"s saving and spending circuits so precisely that they have been able to stir up the saving and disable the spending in some people. The result: people"s preferences switch from spending like a drunken sailor to saving like a child of the Depression. All told, the gray matter responsible for some of our most crucial decisions is finally revealing its secrets.
Psychologists and behavioral economists, meanwhile, are identifying the personality types and other traits that distinguish savers from spenders, showing that people who aren"t good savers are neither stupid nor irrational—but often simply don"t accurately foresee the consequences of not saving. Rewire the brain to find pleasure in future rewards, and you"re on the path to a future you really want.
In one experiment, neuroeconomist Paul Glimcher of New York University wanted to see what it would take for people to willingly delay gratification. He gave a dozen volunteers a choice: $20 now or more money, from $20.25 to $110, later. On one end of the spectrum was the person who agreed to take $21 in a month—to essentially wait a month in order to gain just $1. In economics-speak, this kind of person has a "flat discount function," meaning he values tomorrow almost as much as today and is therefore able to delay gratification. At the other end was someone who was willing to wait a month only if he got $68, a premium of $48 from the original offer. This is someone economists call a "steep discounter," meaning the value he puts on the future (and having money then) is dramatically less than the value he places on today; when he wants something, he wants it now.
单选题
When it comes to spending, new evidence shows that it ______
单选题
The scientists studying spending habits ______
【正确答案】
D
【答案解析】[解析] 参阅上一小题题解。
单选题
If you are rewarded for saving, you are likely to ______
【正确答案】
B
【答案解析】[解析] 第三段提到,科学家发现,许多人乱花钱不是因为他们愚蠢或不理智,而是因为他们不能准确预见乱花钱的后果。如果改变大脑的神经脉络(rewire the brain),让他们看到节省可能带来的好处,那么他们就会向着好的方向发展(即学会不乱花钱)。选择项B基本上表达了这个意思。
单选题
Neuroeconomist Paul Glimcher wants to find out ______
【正确答案】
C
【答案解析】[解析] 第五段第一句提到,神经经济学家Paul Glimcher想弄明白:what it would take for people to willingly delay gratification。他设计了一个实验,结果发现,对未来可能得到的奖赏的看法和期待,决定了人们是否延迟欲望的满足。实际上,最后一段通过描述一个实验,为第四段提到的观点提供了论据。对第四段的理解请参阅上一小题题解。