期刊文献+

自我相关信息的加工优势:来自网名识别的证据 被引量:15

Processing Priority for Self-related Information: Evidence from Visual Search of Screen Names
下载PDF
导出
摘要 以网名为材料,通过三项视觉搜索实验考察了与自我相关的网络信息可能存在的加工优势。结果发现,自己的网名在作为靶刺激时可以更快且更准确地被探测出来;而在作为干扰刺激时,其对于靶刺激却并未表现出比对照刺激更强的抑制作用。在与真实人名进行的直接比较中,自己的网名与真实人名的加工成绩未出现显著差异,且都好于作为对照的名人名字。这些结果证明了与自我相关的网络信息具有和物理世界中的自我信息相似的加工优势,且与以真实人名为材料的多项实验结果完全一致,从而表明自己的网名与真实人名可能具有相同的加工机制。 It is well established in a number of studies that there exists a cognitive priority for self-related material, such as one's own face and name. Among these findings is the well-known cocktail party effect which demonstrates that one's own name can be more easily detected, even in unattended conditions. As both names and faces are fixed self-related material to a large extent, the current research was aimed to extend previous findings by employing a kind of flexible self-related material --- one's own screen name which is usually chosen or designed by users themselves and can be easily changed. Despite the large difference in formation rule, usage practice and environment, screen names and real names share the same function as symbol of self-representation in their respective contexts. As a result, it can be expected there might be a similar cognitive priority for one's own screen name. In the current research, three visual search experiments were designed to examine such a possibility. Experiment 1 compared the visual search speed and accuracy for participants' own QQ (one of the most popular instant messaging software in China, which has several hundreds of million registered users) name and one control QQ name. The results showed that across all three different display set sizes (2, 6 and 12 names), the search speed was consistently faster for participants' own QQ name than control stimuli. Besides that, there was a marginally higher hit rate for own QQ name while the false alarm rates demonstrated no difference under the two conditions. In Experiment 2 which was aimed to examine the own QQ name's interference effect on target item, participants were asked to search one specific QQ name with own/control QQ names as distracters in different trials. ANOVA found neither significant main effects nor interaction. Across all three display set sizes, the search speed and accuracy showed no difference whether own or control QQ name was used as distracters. There was either no difference between hit rates in the two kinds of trials. Experiment 3 made direct comparisons between visual search performance for own screen name and real name. There was no significant difference in either response time or accuracy for these two names. However, they were both more accurately and quickly detected than famous names. Taken together, results of the three experiments showed that as target stimuli, own screen name can be detected more quickly and accurately than control screen name while as distracters, they do not show any larger interference effect on target stimuli. These results completely replicated those of studies using real name stimuli. These similar patterns suggested that recognition of own screen names might share the same mechanism as own real name. In conclusion, the current research provided preliminary evidences on the cognitive priority ot selt-retatect online material. As a result of its self-relevance and repeated use, one's own screen name obtains strong personal salience and the consequent preferential processing. Based on the existence of such an online cocktail party effect, processing priority about self-related information can be extended from real world to the virtual online environment. In future studies, the possible individual difference and the effect of familiarity in screen name recognition constitutes two important issues which deserves to be directly explored.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2012年第4期489-497,共9页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 重庆市人文社会科学重点研究基地重点项目(09SKB26) 中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金项目(XDJK2009C187) 西南大学211工程国家重点学科建设基金项目(NSKD08012) 西南大学博士基金项目(SWU109040)资助
关键词 自我相关的信息 网名 鸡尾酒会效应 加工优势 self-related information screen name cocktail-party effect processing priority
  • 相关文献

参考文献24

  • 1Back,M.D,Schmukle,S.C,Egloff,B. How extraverted is honey.bunny77@hotmail.de? Inferring personality from e-mail addresses[J].Journal of Research in Personality,2008,(04):1116-1122.
  • 2Bechar-Israeli,H. From《 bonehead》 to《 cLonehead》:nicknames,play,and identity on Internet relay chat[J].JOURNAL OF COMPUTER-MEDIATED COMMUNICATION,1995,(02).
  • 3Brédart,S,Delchambre,M,Laureys,S. One's own face is hard to ignore[J].Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,2006,(01):46-52.
  • 4Bundesen,C,Kyllingsbaek,S,Houmann,K.J,Jensen,R.M. Is visual attention automatically attracted by one's own name[J].Perception and Psychophysics,1997,(05):714-720.
  • 5Conway,A.R.A,Cowan,N,Bunting,M.F. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited:The importance of working memory capacity[J].Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,2001,(02):331-335.
  • 6Di,H.B,Yu,S.M,Weng,X.C,Laureys,S Yu,D Li,J.Q. Cerebral response to patient's own name in the vegetative and minimally conscious states[J].Neurology,2007,(12):895-899.doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000258544.79024.d0.
  • 7Devue,C,Brédart,S. Attention to self-referential stimuli:Can I ignore my own face[J].Acta Psychologica,2008,(02):290-297.
  • 8Harris,C.R,Pashler,H. Attention and the processing of emotional words and names:Not so special after all[J].Psychological Science,2004,(03):171-178.doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503005.x.
  • 9Harris,C.R,Pashler,H.E,Coburn,P. Moray revisited:High-priority affective stimuli and visual search[J].The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A,2004,(01):1-31.
  • 10Lieberman,M.D. Social cognitive neuroscience:A review of core processes[J].Annual Review of Psychology,2007,(0):259-289.doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654.

同被引文献123

引证文献15

二级引证文献26

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部