期刊文献+

工作记忆训练提升幼儿流体智力表现 被引量:22

Improvements in Children's Fluid Intelligence with Working Memory Training
下载PDF
导出
摘要 目前已有许多研究证据表明,工作记忆训练能提高成人、儿童的流体智力成绩,然而这种训练是否能够提高幼儿的流体智力表现,更为重要的是,如果训练确有成效,那么这种训练效果能否长期保持?为此,本研究选择了幼儿园4~5岁幼儿进行工作记忆训练任务,考察工作记忆训练对流体智力的提升及保持效应。实验中设立3个组:实验组、控制1组和控制2组。实验组采用单个空间n-back的工作记忆游戏程序进行训练,控制1组采用“水果忍者”的游戏程序进行训练,两组进行游戏训练的时间均为14天,每天15min;控制2组不进行任何训练。结果发现,训练后实验组幼儿被试的流体智力成绩明显优于两个控制组,在6个月后再进行测试,这种优势仍然保持。本研究结果表明,n-back工作记忆训练可以提高幼儿的流体智力成绩,并且这种训练效果具有持续的稳定性。 Fluid intelligence is one of the general intelligence types originally proposed by Cattell (1963), which refers to the innate ability of analytically solving novel problems and logically identifying new patterns and relationships. Increasing evidence has shown that cognitive training, especially those aiming at enhancing working memory, can significantly improve fluid intelligence. Moreover, central executive functions, especially working memory updating, were reported to positively correlate with individual intelligence. Thus, it is of great theoretical and practical significance to investigate whether working memory training can improve fluid intelligence. Recent studies have shown that, after short-term working memory training, performance in fluid intelligence tests, including running memory task and n-back paradigm, was improved both in adults and school-aged children. It is therefore suggested that the transfer of an improved working memory updating ability contributed to the reported training effects. However, there remain 2 major unsolved problems. On the one hand, although training has been reported effective for adults and school-aged children, few studies have focused on pre-school children. On the other hand, the demonstration of long-term effects was unreliable, because the reported studies either failed to examine lasting effects or lacked time for confirmation. In our study, 96 children aged 4-5 from one kindergarten participated in the experiment. They were randomly selected from 3 classes and assigned to 3 groups - experimental group, control group 1, and control group 2. Participants in experimental group were trained for 15 minutes per day for a period of 14 days using an n-back working memory training program displayed on tablet computers. In this program, participants were shown a series of stimuli and asked to judge whether the current stimulus is the same as the one displayed before the previous n items. As n increased, the task difficulty increased, resulting in higher demands of working memory. Each participant started training at 1-back level with n being continuously adjusted based on their performance. To account for the possibility that better fluid IQ performance is essentially due to attention improvement, participants in control group 1 played the Fruit Ninja game for the same period of time, which was designed to yield comparable attention improvements. For control group 2, children received no training. Parallel versions of Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) and the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-4) were used to assess participants' fluid intelligence in pretest, posttest and a follow-up test 6 months after training respectively. All experimental manipulations and the data analysis were designed and performed double-blinded. Results indicated that, after 14-day training, participants' task performance in the training task was significantly improved. Children in experimental group significantly outperformed 2 other control groups in the posttest, which remained equally significant 6 months after training. As task motivation and attitude were balanced, our results were convincing in demonstrating training effects. In conclusion, our results proved that the n-back working memory task can efficiently improve kindergarten children's fluid intelligence, whose effect is sustainable in the long term.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2014年第10期1498-1508,共11页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 国家自然科学基金项目(项目号:31170997) 国家基础人才培养基金:科研训练及科研能力提高(项目号:J1310031)资助
关键词 工作记忆训练 刷新功能 流体智力 幼儿 working memory training working memory updating fluid intelligence kindergarten children
  • 相关文献

参考文献75

  • 1Abu-Rabia, S. (2003). The influence of working memory on reading and creative writing processes in a second language. Educational Psychology, 23(2): 209-222.
  • 2Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Vorking" memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10): 829-839.
  • 3Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89.
  • 4Bergman Nutley, S., S6derqvist, S., Bryde, S., Thorell, L. B., Humphreys, K., & Klingberg, T. (2011). Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: A controlled, randomized study. Developmental Science, 14(3): 591-601.
  • 5Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129(3): 387-398.
  • 6Brown, L., Sherbenou, i. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (2010). TONI-4, Test of Nonverbal Intelligence: Pro-Ed.
  • 7j Brydges, C. R., Reid, C. L., Fox, A. M., & Anderson, M. (2012). A unitary executive function predicts intelligence in children. Intelligence, 40(5): 458-469.
  • 8Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3): 404-431.
  • 9Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1): 1-22.
  • 10Chen, T., & Li, D. (2007). The roles of working memory updating and processing speed in mediating age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(6): 631-646.

二级参考文献100

  • 1陈天勇,李德明.执行功能可分离性及与年龄关系的潜变量分析[J].心理学报,2005,37(2):210-217. 被引量:64
  • 2郭春彦.工作记忆:一个备受关注的研究领域[J].心理科学进展,2007,15(1):1-2. 被引量:31
  • 3Aekerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30-60.
  • 4American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
  • 5Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.
  • 6Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 11, 851-864.
  • 7Barrouillet, P., Mignon, M., & Thevenot, C. (2008). Strategies in subtraction problem solving in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 233-251.
  • 8Caggiano, D. M., Jiang, Y., & Parasuraman, R. (2006). Aging and repetition priming for targets and distracters in a working memory task. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 552-573.
  • 9Camos, V. (2008). Low working memory capacity impedes both efficiency and learning of number transcoding in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 37-57.
  • 10Collette, F., & Van der Linden, M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 105-125.

共引文献107

同被引文献184

引证文献22

二级引证文献62

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部